Self-regulation Strategies to Improve Mathematical Problem Solving for Students with Learning Disabilities

نویسنده

  • Marjorie Montague
چکیده

This article provides a review of research in cognitive strategy instruction for improving mathematical problem solving for students with learning disabilities (LD). The particular focus is on one of the salient components of this instructional approach self-regulation. Seven studies utilizing this approach for teaching problem solving to students with LD were previously evaluated to determine its status as evidence-based practice. The results of this evaluation are described, and the self-regulation component embedded in the cognitive routine for each of the studies is presented. The article concludes with a discussion of several principles associated with research and practice in strategy instruction and some practical considerations for implementation in schools. MARJORIE MONTAGUE, Ph.D., University of Miami. This article provides a review of research in strategy instruction for improving mathematical problem solving for students with learning disabilities (LD) with a focus on one of the salient components of this instructional approach self-regulation. Research has consistently shown that students with LD are poor self-regulators who benefit from strategy instruction that incorporates self-regulation training (Graham & Harris, 2003; Wong, Harris, Graham, & Butler, 2003). Self-regulation, the ability to regulate one's cognitive activities, underlies the executive processes and functions associated with metacognition (Flavell, 1976). Metacognition has to do with knowledge and awareness of one's cognitive strengths and weaknesses as well as self-regulation, which guides an individual in the coordination of that awareness while engaged in cognitive activities (Wong, 1999). Self-regulation strategies, such as self-instruction, self-questioning, self-monitoring, self-evaluation, and self-reinforcement, help learners gain access to cognitive processes that facilitate learning, guide learners as they apply the processes within and across domains, and regulate their application and overall performance of a task. Swanson's (Swanson, 1999; Swanson & Sachs-Lee, 2000) meta-analyses of 30 years of both group and single-subject intervention studies conducted with students with LD revealed that direct instruction and strategy instruction were the two most effective instructional approaches, particularly when combined, for teaching students with LD across academic domains (i.e., reading, writing, and mathematics). Interventions were considered direct instruction if they contained the following components: (a) drills and probes, (b) repeated feedback, (c) rapidly paced instruction, (d) individualized instruction, (e) breaking the task down into a sequence of steps, (f) pictorial diagrams, (g) small-group instruction, and (h) direct questioning by the teacher (Swanson, 1999). In contrast, strategy instruction focuses on processes; for example, metacognition or self-regulation. The following procedures characterized strategy instruction: (a) systematic and direct explanations and/or verbal Volume 31, Winter 2008 37 descriptions of the performance of a task; (b) verbal modeling, questioning, and demonstrations by the teacher of the steps and processes in the cognitive routine; (c) systematic prompts and cues to use the processes, strategies, and procedures; and (d) cognitive modeling using "think aloud" to model task completion or problem solving (Swanson, 1999). Although the two instructional approaches were found to operate independently, they share many components and procedures, such as drill and repetition, distributed practice, task analysis, small-group instruction, and strategy cues, all of which were found to increase the predictive power of treatment effectiveness. Direct instruction was associated more with effective instruction for teaching basic skills such as decoding and math fact recall, as opposed to strategy instruction, which was associated more with effective instruction in higher order learning (e.g., reading comprehension and mathematical problem solving) that utilized higher order skills such as metacognition, self-monitoring, rule learning, and self-awareness (Swanson, 1999; Swanson & Sachs-Lee, 2000). Likewise, Kroesbergen and van Luit (2003), in their meta-analysis of mathematics intervention studies conducted with students with disabilities, found that selfinstruction, a self-regulation strategy, as a component of instructional models, is most effective generally for mathematics learning, but direct instruction appeared more effective for basic skills acquisition. Following a comprehensive search of the literature, seven intervention studies were located that investigated the effects of cognitive strategy instruction on mathematical problem solving for students with disabilities. The five single-subject design and two groupdesign studies were evaluated individually using previously identified quality indicators to determine whether they qualified as "high quality" or "acceptable" and then to determine if the instructional practice, in this case, cognitive strategy instruction for improving mathematical problem solving, qualified as "evidencebased" or "promising" (Gersten et al., 2005; Horner et al., 2005). For the single-subject studies, the benchmarks included (Horner et al., 2005): 1. Sufficient description of the participants and setting 2. Sufficient description of the measures and measurment procedures, including interrater agreeement 3. Sufficient description of the intervention and procedures for determining fidelity of implementation 4. Sufficient description of the baseline phase and evidence of a pattern prior to intervention 5. At least three demonstrations of experimental effect, explanations of how internal and external validity were controlled, and established social importance and cost-effectiveness of the intervention For the group-design studies, the benchmarks included (Gersten et al., 2005): 1. Research based on previous studies or a compelling argument for its importance 2. Sufficient description of the participants, settings attrition, and intervention agents 3. Sufficient description of the intervention, procedures for determining fidelity of implementation, and differences between treatment and control groups 4. Sufficient description of the measures and technical adequacy and data collection procedures 5. Sufficient description of the analytic procedures with emphasis on the power analysis, unit of analysis, and variability in the sample These studies were then reviewed using the benchmarks to determine the quality of the research and, ultimately, to draw conclusions as to whether cognitive strategy instruction is evidence-based or at least promising (Montague & Dietz, in press). The remainder of this article provides a summary of the results of the review, describes the self-regulation component embedded in the cognitive routine for each of the studies, reviews several principles associated with research in strategy instruction, and offers some guidelines for implementation. Results of the Literature Review Montague and Dietz (in press) evaluated five singlesubject studies: Montague and Bos (1986); Case, Harris, and Graham (1992); Montague (1992); Hutchinson (1993); and Cassel and Reid (1996); and two group studies: Montague, Applegate, and Marquard (1993); and Chung and Tam (2005). The studies were rated by three independent raters to determine (a) whether each study met each of the quality indicators listed above; (b) whether each individual study met the criteria for "high quality" research; and (c) whether, as a body of work, the research met the standards for deeming the practice "evidence-based." Single-subject design studies. For the single-design studies to meet the standards, the body of research must have included at least five studies that met minimally acceptable methodological criteria, documented experimental control, appeared in peer-reviewed journals, were conducted by at least three different researchers across at least three geographical locations, and had at least 20 participants across studies. When applying the standards and criteria developed by Horner et al. (2005) to evaluate the quality of the research, the five single-subject design studies stood up well. All used researcher-developed interventions. Learning Disability Quaiterly 38 which, although similar in many respects, varied somewhat with regard to the cognitive and metacognitive components. All interventions produced positive outcomes for individual students. Performance improved, although some students did not meet the criterion for mastery. Most students showed maintenance over time and maintained use of the strategy in classroom settings. However, there was evidence that performance declined over time without distributed review and practice. An overall analysis of the studies as a group concluded that the practice cognitive strategy instruction is evidence-based and does improve mathematical problem solving for students with mathematical disabilities. Group-design studies. For the two group-design studies to meet the standards, the body of research must have included at least four acceptable studies or two high-quality studies that supported the practice. In addition, to be considered evidence-based, the weighted effect size must have been significantly greater than zero; for "promising," there must have been at least a 20% confidence interval for the weighted effect size that was greater than zero. The two group studies did not meet the criteria for either evidence-based or promising practice due to methodological issues. The primary problems for both studies included a lack of procedures to measure treatment fidelity and limited information regarding the technical adequacy of the outcome measures. This suggests that group studies designed to test the effectiveness of this practice need to be more rigorous and designed with the quality indicators in mind. All raters agreed that the interventions for both studies were described clearly and the results were positive. Self-Regulation Components of Cognitive Strategy Instruction The goal of cognitive strategy instruction is to teach learners multiple cognitive and metacognitive processes and strategies to facilitate and enhance performance in academic domains (e.g., mathematical problem solving) as well as nonacademic domains (e.g., social problem solving). The processes and strategies range from simple to complex depending on task difficulty and context of the task. Students with LD characteristically are poor strategic learners and problem solvers and manifest strategy deficits and differences that impede performance, particularly on tasks requiring higher level processing. These students need explicit instruction in selecting strategies appropriate to the task, applying the strategies in the context of the task, and monitoring their execution. They have difficulty abandoning and replacing ineffective strategies, adapting strategies to other similar tasks, and generalizing strategies to other situations and settings. Instruction aims to develop strategic learners who have an effective and efficient repertoire of strategies and are motivated, self-directed, and selfregulating. In contrast to direct instruction, which is didactic and grounded in behaviorism, the theoretical foundation of cognitive strategy instruction considers both behavioral and cognitive theory; that is, information processing and developmental theory. Instruction focuses on cognitive processes, such as visualization, and metacognitive or self-regulation strategies, such as self-questioning. Cognitive strategy instruction teaches students to think and behave like good problem solvers and strategic learners. A cognitive routine is taught using explicit instruction, an instructional model that consists of very structured and organized lessons, appropriate cues and prompts, guided and distributed practice, cognitive modeling, interaction between teachers and students, immediate and corrective feedback on performance, positive reinforcement, overlearning, and mastery. All the studies included in Montague and Dietz's (in press) review focused on teaching a specific cognitive routine for mathematical problem solving that includes a self-regulation component. The studies included a total of 142 students ranging in age from 8-4 to 16-7 years. Most of the participants were identified with learning disabilities (N = 110), while two identified participants as having mild intellectual disabilities (Cassel & Reid, Chung & Tam, 2005). Montague used additional preset criteria for participation that included average intelligence, at least a third-grade reading level, and facility with the four basic math operations using whole numbers and decimals. Montague et al (1986, 1992, 1993). Montague's cognitive routine (Montague & Bos, 1986; Montague, 1992; Montague et al., 1993) is a seven-phase model with specific self-regulation components. In the 1986 study, self-regulation was embedded in a script; for example, A self-questioning technique such as "What is asked?" or "What am I looking for?" was used to provide focus on the outcome. The two later studies specified a SAY, ASK, CHECK routine for each of the seven processes taught. SAY requires students to self-instruct, which helps students identify and direct themselves as they solve the problem. For example, when reading the problem, students SAY "Read the problem. If I don't understand it, read it again." ASK refers to self-questioning, which promotes internal dialogue that helps to systematically analyze the problem information and regulate execution of the cognitive processes. When students paraphrase the problem, they ASK themselves, "Have I underlined the Volume 31, Winter 2008 39 important information? What is the question? What am I looking for?" Finally, CHECK is the self-monitoring strategy that promotes appropriate use of specific strategies and encourages students to monitor their performance throughout the problem solving process. When students formulate a visual representation of the problem, they CHECK "the picture against the problem information." Figure 1 presents the entire routine the seven processes and the corresponding SAY, ASK, CHECK component for each. Students are required to memorize the processes and become familiar with the selfregulation component. After students understand what the processes are and can recite them from memory, the teacher uses process or cognitive modeling to demonstrate how good problem solvers approach a mathematical problem. Students are then required to "think aloud" as they solve practice problems. Finally, they become the "teacher," modeling how good problem solvers think and behave. In the two single-subject studies (Montague & Bos, 1986; Montague, 1992), the strategy use of six secondary and six middle school students improved substantially, and strategy maintenance was evident. However, the two sixth graders did not meet the mastery criterion, suggesting that the comprehensive cognitive routine may have been developmentally beyond their ability. For the group study, 72 middle school students were taught in groups of 8-12; on the posttest, they performed to the level of a group of nondisabled students. Graham and Harris (2003). The Self-Regulated Strategy Development model (SRSD; Graham & Harris, 2003), designed in the early 1980s to improve composition skills of students with LD, was the basis for the intervention studies by Case et al. (1992) and Cassel and Reid (1996). This model includes the basic components of all cognitive strategy instructional routines. The model consists of six stages to guide instruction: (a) develop and activate background knowledge by providing the knowledge and skills needed to acquire and apply strategies and procedures for problem solving, (b) discuss the strategy by looking at the student's current performance and explaining the strategies and how they will help the student improve their problem solving, (c) model the strategy using "think aloud" to demonstrate how giving oneself instructions helps regulate strategy use during problem solving, (d) have students memorize the strategy steps and self-statements, (e) support strategy use by providing guided practice using scaffolded instructional techniques, and (f) monitor students' performance until they can use the specific math problemsolving and self-regulation strategies independently. Case et al. (1992). The variation of the SRSD model in the study by Case et al. (1992) included preskill development; conferencing regarding each student's current performance level, metastrategy information, and commitment to learning the strategy; discussing the problem-solving strategy; modeling the strategy and self-instructions; mastery of the strategy steps; collaboratively practicing the strategy and self-instructions; independent performance; and generalization and maintenance components. As part of the package, instructional goals were set coUaboratively by the student and the teacher, followed by a discussion of the importance of the strategy and the self-regulation strategies (self-assessment, self-recording, and selfinstruction). The strategy was introduced using a small chart listing the following five steps: 1. Read the problem out loud. 2. Look for important words and circle them. 3. Draw pictures to help tell what is happening. 4. Write down the math sentence. 5. Write down the answer. Four students with LD in grades 5 and 6 progressed from learning to apply the strategy with simple addition problems to subtraction problems. Students' performance on the addition problems remained high after instruction. On the subtraction problems, student performance increased dramatically, and students were able to discriminate between addition and subtraction problems, thus minimizing selection of the wrong operation. Cassel and Reid (1996). Cassel and Reid (1996) used similar procedures to teach the strategy: preskill development, initial conference, discussion of the problem-solving strategy and self-regulation procedures, modeling the strategy and self-instructions, strategy mastery, collaborative practice, independent practice, and maintenance. The strategy consisted of the following nine steps and the acronym "FAST DRAW." l.Read the problem out loud. 2. Find and highlight the question, then write the label. 3. Ask what are the parts of the problem, then circle the numbers needed. 4. Set up the problem by writing and labeling the numbers. 5. Reread the problem and tie down the sign (decide if you use addition or subtraction). 6. Discover the sign (recheck the operation). 7. Read the number problem. 8. Answer the number problem. 9. Write the answer and check by asking if the answer makes sense. The teacher modeled strategy use using self-talk and self-questioning; for example, "What is it I have to do?" Learning Disability Quarterly 40 Figure 1. Math problem-solving processes and strategies. READ (for understanding) Say: Read the problem. If I don't understand, read it again. Ask: Have I read and understood the problenn? Check: For understanding as I solve the problem. PARAPHRASE (your own words) Say: Underline the important information. Put the problem in my own words. Ask: Have I underlined the important information? What is the question? What am I looking for? Check: That the information goes with the question. VISUALIZE (a picture or a diagram) Say: Make a drawing or a diagram. Show the relationships among the problem parts. Ask: Does the picture fit the problem? Did I show the relationships? Check: The picture against the problem information. HYPOTHESIZE (a plan to solve the problem) Say: Decide how many steps and operations are needed. Write the operation symbols (+, -, x, and /). Ask: If I ..., what will I get? If I ..., then what do I need to do next? How many steps

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

Comparison of Children with Attention Deficit / Hyperactivity Disorder, Special Learning Disorder and normal in Sensory Processing, Academic Help Seeking and Problem-Solving Strategies.

Objectives: The present study was conducted for evaluate and compare sensory processing, academic help seeking and problem-solving strategies in children with Attention Deficit / Hyperactivity Disorder, special learning disabilities and normal students. Method: In the present study, which was of a comparative scientific type, among all 10 to 12 years old students with attention deficit / hypera...

متن کامل

The Effects of Learning Self-Regulated Learning Strategies on High School Students\' Math Problem-Solving Skills

The Effects of Learning Self-Regulated Learning Strategies on High School Students' Math Problem-Solving Skills B. Shaalchi, Ph.D. Y. Rezaapoor J. Baabaapoor Kheyrodin, Ph.D. M. Alizaad Sardroodi F. Abdollaahi To see the effects of learning self-regulated learning strategies on math problem-solving skills among high school students, a sample of 271 randomly selected h...

متن کامل

The Effectiveness of Cognitive Rehabilitation on Mathematical Word Problem Solving in Students with Specefic Learning Disability with Impairment in Mathematic

The aim of the current reaerch was to study the effectiveness of cognitive rehabilitation on mathematical word problem solving in students with specific learning disabilitiy with impairment in mathematic. The research design was quasi-experimental pre-test and post-test with a control group. The statistical population of the study included all male students with specefic learning disabilitiy wi...

متن کامل

compare the effectiveness of teaching strategy learning and visualization and self-regulation training on student problem solving skills

Background and Aim: The purpose of this study was to compare the effectiveness of teaching strategy learning and visualization and self-regulation training on student problem solving skills. Materials and Methods: The present research was experimental. The research population consisted of all 7th grade students in Tehran during the academic year 1397-1396. Using multi-stage cluster sampling, 12...

متن کامل

Reducing the academic burnout of students with learning disabilities through attributional retraining, emotional regulation, and cognitive-social problem solving

objective: The aim of this research was to investigate and compare the effectiveness of attributional retraining program, emotion regulation and cognitive-social problem solving on reduction of academic burnout in students with early learning disabilities in Tehran. Method: This was a quasi-experimental study with pretest-posttest,and follow up. The participants consisted of 40 students with le...

متن کامل

Effectiveness of emotion regulation training in improving self perception and assertiveness of students with learning disabilities

The research aim was to investigate effectiveness of emotion regulation training in improving self perception and assertiveness of students with learning disabilities. Research method was quasi-experimental with pretest, posttest, and control group. Statistical population consisted students with learning disabilities who had been referred by the schools to the center for learning disabilities o...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2008